Paola Arlotta: Brain Development from Stem Cell to Organoid #32

Transcript

00:00:00 The following is a conversation with Paola Arlotta.

00:00:03 She’s a professor of stem cell and regenerative biology

00:00:06 at Harvard University and is interested in understanding

00:00:09 the molecular laws that govern the birth, differentiation,

00:00:13 and assembly of the human brain’s cerebral cortex.

00:00:16 She explores the complexity of the brain

00:00:18 by studying and engineering elements

00:00:21 of how the brain develops.

00:00:22 This was a fascinating conversation to me.

00:00:25 It’s part of the Artificial Intelligence podcast.

00:00:28 If you enjoy it, subscribe on YouTube,

00:00:30 give it five stars on iTunes, support it on Patreon,

00:00:33 or simply connect with me on Twitter

00:00:35 at Lex Friedman, spelled F R I D M A N.

00:00:39 And I’d like to give a special thank you to Amy Jeffress

00:00:43 for her support of the podcast on Patreon.

00:00:45 She’s an artist and you should definitely check out

00:00:47 her Instagram at lovetruthgood, three beautiful words.

00:00:52 Your support means a lot and inspires me

00:00:55 to keep the series going.

00:00:57 And now here’s my conversation with Paola Arlotta.

00:01:03 You studied the development of the human brain

00:01:05 for many years.

00:01:06 So let me ask you an out of the box question first.

00:01:11 How likely is it that there’s intelligent life out there

00:01:14 in the universe outside of earth

00:01:17 with something like the human brain?

00:01:19 So I can put it another way.

00:01:20 How unlikely is the human brain?

00:01:24 How difficult is it to build a thing

00:01:28 through the evolutionary process?

00:01:30 Well, it has happened here, right?

00:01:32 On this planet.

00:01:33 Once, yes.

00:01:34 Once.

00:01:36 So that simply tells you that it could, of course,

00:01:39 happen again other places.

00:01:42 It’s only a matter of probability.

00:01:44 What the probability that you would get a brain

00:01:46 like the ones that we have, like the human brain.

00:01:51 So how difficult is it to make the human brain?

00:01:53 It’s pretty difficult, but most importantly,

00:01:59 I guess we know very little

00:02:00 about how this process really happens.

00:02:04 And there is a reason for that,

00:02:06 actually multiple reasons for that.

00:02:09 Most of what we know about how the mammalian brain,

00:02:13 so the brain of mammals develop comes from studying

00:02:17 in labs other brains, not our own brain,

00:02:20 the brain of mice, for example.

00:02:22 But if I showed you a picture of a mouse brain,

00:02:25 and then you put it next to a picture of a human brain,

00:02:28 they don’t look at all like each other.

00:02:31 So they’re very different.

00:02:32 And therefore there is a limit to what you can learn

00:02:36 about how the human brain is made

00:02:37 by studying the mouse brain.

00:02:41 There is a huge value in studying the mouse brain.

00:02:43 There are many things that we have learned,

00:02:45 but it’s not the same thing.

00:02:46 So in having studied the human brain,

00:02:49 or through the mouse and through other methodologies

00:02:51 that we’ll talk about, do you have a sense?

00:02:54 I mean, you’re one of the experts in the world.

00:02:57 How much do you feel you know about the brain

00:03:01 and how often do you find yourself

00:03:05 in awe of this mysterious thing?

00:03:07 Yeah, you pretty much find yourself in awe all the time.

00:03:12 It’s an amazing process.

00:03:15 It’s a process by which,

00:03:17 by means that we don’t fully understand,

00:03:20 at the very beginning of embryogenesis,

00:03:23 the structure called the neural tube,

00:03:26 literally self assembles.

00:03:28 And it happens in an embryo

00:03:30 and it can happen also from stem cells in a dish.

00:03:33 Okay.

00:03:34 And then from there,

00:03:36 these stem cells that are present within the neural tube

00:03:39 give rise to all of the thousands and thousands

00:03:42 of different cell types that are present in the brain

00:03:45 through time, right?

00:03:46 With the interesting, very intriguing, interesting

00:03:50 observation is that the time that it takes

00:03:54 for the human brain to be made, it’s human time.

00:03:58 Meaning that for me and you,

00:04:02 it took almost nine months of gestation to build the brain

00:04:05 and then another 20 years of learning postnatally

00:04:08 to get the brain that we have today

00:04:09 that allows us to this conversation.

00:04:12 A mouse takes 20 days or so for an embryo to be born.

00:04:19 And so the brain is built in a much shorter period of time.

00:04:23 And the beauty of it is that if you take mouse stem cells

00:04:27 and you put them in a culture dish,

00:04:29 the brain organoid that you get from a mouse

00:04:33 is formed faster than if you took human stem cells

00:04:37 and put them in the dish

00:04:39 and let them make a human brain organoid.

00:04:41 So the very developmental process is…

00:04:45 Controlled by the speed of the species.

00:04:49 Which means it’s on purpose, it’s not accidental

00:04:54 or there is something in that temporal…

00:04:58 It’s very, exactly, that is very important

00:05:01 for us to get the brain we have.

00:05:04 And we can speculate for why that is.

00:05:08 You know, it takes us a long time as human beings

00:05:11 after we’re born to learn all the things

00:05:14 that we have to learn to have the adult brain.

00:05:17 It’s actually 20 years, think about it.

00:05:20 From when a baby is born to when a teenager

00:05:23 goes through puberty to adults, it’s a long time.

00:05:27 Do you think you can maybe talk through

00:05:30 the first few months and then on to the first 20 years

00:05:35 and then for the rest of their lives?

00:05:37 What is the development of the human brain look like?

00:05:41 What are the different stages?

00:05:42 Yeah, at the beginning, you have to build a brain, right?

00:05:46 And the brain is made of cells.

00:05:48 What’s the very beginning?

00:05:49 Which beginning are we talking about?

00:05:52 In the embryo, as the embryo is developing in the womb,

00:05:56 in addition to making all of the other tissues

00:05:58 of the embryo, the muscle, the heart, the blood,

00:06:02 the embryo is also building the brain.

00:06:04 And it builds from a very simple structure

00:06:08 called the neural tube, which is basically nothing

00:06:11 but a tube of cells that spans sort of the length

00:06:14 of the embryo from the head all the way to the tail,

00:06:18 let’s say, of the embryo.

00:06:20 And then over in human beings, over many months of gestation

00:06:25 from that neural tube, which contains stem cell

00:06:30 like cells of the brain, you will make many, many

00:06:35 other building blocks of the brain.

00:06:36 So all of the other cell types, because there are many,

00:06:40 many different types of cells in the brain

00:06:43 that will form specific structures of the brain.

00:06:46 So you can think about embryonic development of the brain

00:06:49 as just the time in which you are making

00:06:51 the building blocks, the cells.

00:06:54 Are the stem cells relatively homogeneous, like uniform,

00:06:57 or are they all different types?

00:06:59 It’s a very good question.

00:07:00 It’s exactly how it works.

00:07:01 You start with a more homogeneous,

00:07:04 perhaps more multipotent type of stem cell.

00:07:09 With multipotent.

00:07:10 With multipotent it means that it has the potential

00:07:13 to make many, many different types of other cells.

00:07:17 And then with time, these progenitors become

00:07:20 more heterogeneous, which means more diverse.

00:07:22 There are gonna be many different types of the stem cells.

00:07:26 And also they will give rise to progeny to other cells

00:07:30 that are not stem cells, that are specific cells

00:07:32 of the brain that are very different

00:07:34 from the mother stem cell.

00:07:35 And now you think about this process of making cells

00:07:38 from the stem cells over many, many months

00:07:41 of development for humans.

00:07:43 And what you’re doing, you’re building the cells

00:07:46 that physically make the brain,

00:07:48 and then you arrange them in specific structures

00:07:52 that are present in the final brain.

00:07:55 So you can think about the embryonic development

00:07:58 of the brain as the time where you’re building the bricks,

00:08:02 you’re putting the bricks together to form buildings,

00:08:05 structures, regions of the brain.

00:08:08 And where you make the connections

00:08:10 between these many different type of cells,

00:08:13 especially nerve cells, neurons, right?

00:08:15 That transmit action potentials and electricity.

00:08:19 I’ve heard you also say somewhere, I think,

00:08:21 correct me if I’m wrong,

00:08:22 that the order of the way this builds matters.

00:08:25 Oh yes.

00:08:26 If you are an engineer and you think about development,

00:08:29 you can think of it as, well, I could also take all the cells

00:08:35 and bring them all together into a brain in the end.

00:08:38 But development is much more than that.

00:08:40 So the cells are made in a very specific order

00:08:43 that subserve the final product that you need to get.

00:08:47 And so, for example, all of the nerve cells,

00:08:49 the neurons are made first,

00:08:52 and all of the supportive cells of the neurons,

00:08:54 like the glia, is made later.

00:08:56 And there is a reason for that

00:08:58 because they have to assemble together in specific ways.

00:09:02 But you also may say, well,

00:09:03 why don’t we just put them all together in the end?

00:09:05 It’s because as they develop next to each other,

00:09:09 they influence their own development.

00:09:11 So it’s a different thing for a glia

00:09:13 to be made alone in a dish,

00:09:15 than a glia cell be made in a developing embryo

00:09:19 with all these other cells around it

00:09:21 that produce all these other signals.

00:09:23 First of all, that’s mind blowing,

00:09:25 this development process.

00:09:27 From my perspective in artificial intelligence,

00:09:29 you often think of how incredible the final product is,

00:09:33 the final product, the brain.

00:09:35 But you’re making me realize that the final product

00:09:38 is just, the beautiful thing

00:09:42 is the actual development process.

00:09:44 Do we know the code that drives that development?

00:09:51 Yeah.

00:09:52 Do we have any sense?

00:09:53 First of all, thank you for saying

00:09:55 that it’s really the formation of the brain.

00:09:59 It’s really its development.

00:10:00 It is this incredibly choreographed dance

00:10:05 that happens the same way every time

00:10:07 each one of us builds the brain, right?

00:10:10 And that builds an organ that allows us

00:10:12 to do what we’re doing today, right?

00:10:14 That is mind blowing.

00:10:16 And this is why developmental neurobiologists

00:10:18 never get tired of studying that.

00:10:21 Now you’re asking about the code.

00:10:23 What drives this?

00:10:24 How is this done?

00:10:26 Well, it’s millions of years of evolution

00:10:29 of really fine tuning gene expression programs

00:10:33 that allow certain cells to be made at a certain time

00:10:37 and to become a certain cell type,

00:10:41 but also mechanical forces of pressure bending.

00:10:47 This embryo is not just, it will not stay a tube,

00:10:50 this brain for very long.

00:10:52 At some point, this tube in the front of the embryo

00:10:54 will expand to make the primordium of the brain, right?

00:10:58 Now the forces that control that the cells feel,

00:11:02 and this is another beautiful thing,

00:11:04 the very force that they feel,

00:11:06 which is different from a week before, a week ago,

00:11:10 will tell the cell, oh, you’re being squished

00:11:12 in a certain way, begin to produce these new genes

00:11:16 because now you are at the corner

00:11:18 or you are in a stretch of cells

00:11:21 or whatever it is, and that,

00:11:23 so that mechanical physical force

00:11:26 shapes the fate of the cell as well.

00:11:29 So it’s not only chemical, it’s also mechanical.

00:11:32 So from my perspective,

00:11:34 biology is this incredibly complex mess, gooey mess.

00:11:40 So you’re saying mechanical forces.

00:11:43 How different is like a computer

00:11:47 or any kind of mechanical machine that we humans build

00:11:52 and the biological systems?

00:11:54 Have you been,

00:11:54 because you’ve worked a lot with biological systems.

00:11:57 Are they as much of a mess as it seems

00:12:00 from a perspective of an engineer, a mechanical engineer?

00:12:03 Yeah, they are much more prone

00:12:08 to taking alternative routes, right?

00:12:11 So if you, we go back to printing a brain

00:12:16 versus developing a brain,

00:12:18 of course, if you print a brain,

00:12:20 given that you start with the same building blocks,

00:12:23 the same cells,

00:12:23 you could potentially print it the same way every time,

00:12:28 but that final brain may not work the same way

00:12:32 as a brain built during development does

00:12:34 because the very same building blocks that you’re using

00:12:38 developed in a completely different environment, right?

00:12:41 It was not the environment of the brain.

00:12:43 Therefore, they’re gonna be different just by definition.

00:12:46 So if you instead use development to build,

00:12:50 let’s say a brain organoid,

00:12:52 which maybe we will be talking about in a few minutes.

00:12:55 Those things are fascinating.

00:12:56 Yes, so if you use processes of development,

00:13:01 then when you watch it,

00:13:03 you can see that sometimes things can go wrong

00:13:06 in some organoids and by wrong,

00:13:08 I mean different one organoid from the next.

00:13:10 While if you think about that embryo, it always goes right.

00:13:14 So this development, it’s for as complex as it is.

00:13:18 Every time a baby is born has, with very few exceptions,

00:13:23 so the brain is like the next baby,

00:13:26 but it’s not the same if you develop it in a dish.

00:13:31 And first of all, we don’t even develop a brain,

00:13:33 you develop something much simpler in the dish,

00:13:36 but there are more options for building things differently,

00:13:39 which really tells you that evolution

00:13:42 has played a really tight game here

00:13:48 for how in the end the brain is built in vivo.

00:13:53 So just a quick, maybe dumb question,

00:13:55 but it seems like this is not,

00:13:58 the building process is not a dictatorship.

00:14:01 It seems like there’s not a centralized,

00:14:04 like high level mechanism that says,

00:14:07 okay, this cell built itself the wrong way,

00:14:10 I’m gonna kill it.

00:14:11 It seems like there’s a really strong distributed mechanism.

00:14:15 Is that in your sense for what you mean?

00:14:18 There are a lot of possibilities, right?

00:14:20 And if you think about, for example,

00:14:23 different species building their brain,

00:14:26 each brain is a little bit different.

00:14:28 So the brain of a lizard is very different

00:14:31 from that of a chicken, from that of one of us

00:14:35 and so on and so forth and still is a brain,

00:14:38 but it was built differently starting from stem cells

00:14:43 that pretty much had the same potential,

00:14:45 but in the end, evolution builds different brains

00:14:49 in different species because that serves in a way

00:14:52 the purpose of that species

00:14:53 and the wellbeing of that organism.

00:14:56 And so there are many possibilities,

00:15:00 but then there is a way and you were talking about a code.

00:15:04 Nobody knows what the entire code of development is.

00:15:07 Of course we don’t.

00:15:08 We know bits and pieces of very specific aspects

00:15:13 of development of the brain,

00:15:14 what genes are involved to make a certain cell types,

00:15:17 how those two cells interact to make the next level structure

00:15:20 that we might know, but the entirety of it,

00:15:22 how it’s so well controlled, it’s really mind blowing.

00:15:26 So in the first two months in the embryo or whatever,

00:15:29 the first few weeks, months,

00:15:32 so yeah, the building blocks are constructed.

00:15:37 The actual, the different regions of the brain,

00:15:40 I guess in the nervous system.

00:15:42 Well, this continues way longer

00:15:44 than just the first few months.

00:15:46 So over the very first few months,

00:15:50 you build a lot of the cells,

00:15:52 but then there is continuous building of new cell types

00:15:56 all the way through birth.

00:15:58 And then even postnatally,

00:16:00 I don’t know if you’ve ever heard of myelin.

00:16:03 Myelin is this sort of insulation

00:16:06 that is built around the cables of the neurons

00:16:09 so that the electricity can go really fast from.

00:16:12 The axons, I guess they’re called.

00:16:13 The axons, they’re called axons, exactly.

00:16:15 And so as human beings,

00:16:19 we myelinate our cells postnatally.

00:16:24 A kid, a six year old kid has barely started

00:16:28 the process of making the mature oligodendrocytes,

00:16:31 which are the cells that then eventually

00:16:33 will wrap the axons into myelin.

00:16:36 And this will continue, believe it or not,

00:16:38 until we are about 25, 30 years old.

00:16:42 So there is a continuous process of maturation

00:16:45 and tweaking and additions,

00:16:47 and also in response to what we do.

00:16:51 I remember taking AP Biology in high school,

00:16:53 and in the textbook, it said that,

00:16:57 I’m going by memory here,

00:16:58 that scientists disagree on the purpose

00:17:01 of myelin in the brain.

00:17:04 Is that totally wrong?

00:17:06 So like, I guess it speeds up the,

00:17:12 okay, I might be wrong here,

00:17:13 but I guess it speeds up the electricity

00:17:14 traveling down the axon or something.

00:17:17 Yeah, so that’s the most sort of canonical,

00:17:20 and definitely that’s the case.

00:17:21 So you have to imagine an axon,

00:17:24 and you can think about it as a cable of some type

00:17:27 with electricity going through.

00:17:29 And what myelin does, by insulating the outside,

00:17:34 I should say there are tracts of myelin

00:17:36 and pieces of axons that are naked without myelin.

00:17:39 And so by having the insulation,

00:17:41 the electricity, instead of going straight

00:17:43 through the cable, it will jump

00:17:45 over a piece of myelin, right,

00:17:47 to the next naked little piece and jump again.

00:17:49 And therefore, that’s the idea that you go faster.

00:17:52 And it was always thought that in order to build

00:17:57 a big brain, a big nervous system,

00:18:00 in order to have a nervous system

00:18:03 that can do very complex type of things,

00:18:05 then you need a lot of myelin

00:18:06 because you wanna go fast with this information

00:18:09 from point A to point B.

00:18:12 Well, a few years ago, maybe five years ago or so,

00:18:16 we discovered that some of the most evolved,

00:18:19 which means the newest type of neurons that we have

00:18:23 as nonhuman primates, as human beings

00:18:25 in the top of our cerebral cortex,

00:18:28 which should be the neurons that do some

00:18:29 of the most complex things that we do,

00:18:32 well, those have axons that have very little myelin.

00:18:36 Wow.

00:18:36 And they have very interesting ways

00:18:41 in which they put this myelin on their axons.

00:18:43 You know, a little piece here,

00:18:44 then a long track with no myelin, another chunk there.

00:18:47 And some don’t have myelin at all.

00:18:49 So now, you have to explain

00:18:52 where we’re going with evolution.

00:18:57 And if you think about it,

00:18:58 perhaps as an electrical engineer,

00:19:01 when I looked at it, I initially thought,

00:19:05 and I’m a developmental neurobiologist,

00:19:06 I thought maybe this is what we see now,

00:19:10 but if we give evolution another few million years,

00:19:13 we’ll see a lot of myelin on these neurons too.

00:19:15 But I actually think now that that’s instead the future

00:19:19 of the brain.

00:19:20 Less myelin.

00:19:21 Less myelin might allow for more flexibility

00:19:24 on what you do with your axons,

00:19:26 and therefore more complicated

00:19:28 and unpredictable type of functions,

00:19:31 which is also a bit mind blowing.

00:19:33 So it seems like it’s controlling the timing of the signal.

00:19:37 So they’re in the timing, you can encode a lot of information.

00:19:42 Yeah.

00:19:43 And so the brain.

00:19:44 The timing, the chemistry of that little piece of axon,

00:19:48 perhaps it’s a dynamic process where the myelin can move.

00:19:52 Now you see how many layers of variability you can add,

00:19:57 and that’s actually really good

00:19:58 if you’re trying to come up with a new function

00:20:02 or a new capability or something unpredictable in a way.

00:20:06 So we’re gonna jump around a little bit,

00:20:08 but the old question of how much is nature

00:20:12 and how much is nurture?

00:20:14 In terms of this incredible thing

00:20:17 after the development is over,

00:20:20 we seem to be kind of somewhat smart, intelligent,

00:20:26 cognition, consciousness,

00:20:27 all of these things are just incredible,

00:20:29 ability to reason and so on emerge.

00:20:31 In your sense, how much is in the hardware,

00:20:34 in the nature and how much is in the nurture

00:20:39 is learned through with our parents

00:20:40 through interacting with the environment and so on.

00:20:42 It’s really both, right?

00:20:43 If you think about it.

00:20:45 So we are born with a brain as babies

00:20:48 that has most of its cells and most of its structures.

00:20:53 And that will take a few years to grow,

00:20:57 to add more, to be better.

00:21:00 But really then we have this 20 years

00:21:04 of interacting with the environment around us.

00:21:07 And so what that brain that was so perfectly built

00:21:10 or imperfectly built due to our genetic cues

00:21:16 will then be used to incorporate the environment

00:21:20 in its further maturation and development.

00:21:22 And so your experiences do shape your brain.

00:21:26 I mean, we know that like if you and I

00:21:29 may have had a different childhood or a different,

00:21:32 we have been going to different schools,

00:21:35 we have been learning different things

00:21:36 and our brain is a little bit different because of that.

00:21:38 We behave differently because of that.

00:21:41 And so especially postnatally

00:21:44 experience is extremely important.

00:21:46 We are born with a plastic brain.

00:21:48 What that means is a brain that is able to change

00:21:51 in response to stimuli that can be sensory.

00:21:56 So perhaps some of the most illuminating studies

00:22:01 that were done were studies in which

00:22:03 the sensory organs were not working, right?

00:22:06 Like if you are born with eyes that don’t work,

00:22:09 then your very brain, that piece of the brain

00:22:12 that normally would process vision, the visual cortex,

00:22:17 develops postnatally differently

00:22:19 and it might be used to do something different, right?

00:22:23 So that’s the most extreme.

00:22:25 The plasticity of the brain, I guess,

00:22:27 is the magic hardware that it,

00:22:29 and then it’s flexibility in all forms

00:22:32 is what enables the learning postnatally.

00:22:36 Can you talk about organoids?

00:22:39 What are they?

00:22:40 And how can you use them to help us understand the brain

00:22:44 and the development of the brain?

00:22:45 This is very, very important.

00:22:47 So the first thing I’d like to say,

00:22:49 please skip this in the video.

00:22:52 The first thing I’d like to say is that an organoid,

00:22:56 a brain organoid is not the same as a brain.

00:23:00 Okay?

00:23:01 It’s a fundamental distinction.

00:23:03 It’s a system, a cellular system

00:23:08 that one can develop in the culture dish,

00:23:12 starting from stem cells that will mimic some aspects

00:23:17 of the development of the brain, but not all of it.

00:23:21 They are very small, maximum,

00:23:23 they become about four to five millimeters in diameters.

00:23:27 They are much simpler than our brain, of course,

00:23:32 but yet they are the only system

00:23:35 where we can literally watch a process

00:23:39 of human brain development unfold.

00:23:42 And by watch, I mean, study it.

00:23:44 Remember when I told you that we can’t understand

00:23:47 everything about development in our own brain

00:23:49 by studying a mouse?

00:23:51 Well, we can’t study the actual process

00:23:53 of development of the human brain

00:23:54 because it all happens in utero.

00:23:55 So we will never have access to that process ever.

00:23:58 And therefore, this is our next best thing.

00:24:02 Like a bunch of stem cells that can be coaxed

00:24:06 into starting a process of neural tube formation.

00:24:10 Remember that tube that is made by the embryo early on.

00:24:13 And from there, a lot of the cell types

00:24:15 that are present within the brain,

00:24:19 and you can simply watch it and study,

00:24:23 but you can also think about diseases

00:24:27 where development of the brain

00:24:29 does not proceed normally, right, properly.

00:24:33 Think about neurodevelopmental diseases.

00:24:34 There are many, many different types.

00:24:37 Think about autism spectrum disorders.

00:24:39 There are also many different types of autism.

00:24:41 So there you could take a stem cell,

00:24:44 which really means either a sample of blood

00:24:46 or a sample of skin from the patient,

00:24:50 make a stem cell, and then with that stem cell,

00:24:53 watch a process of formation of a brain organ

00:24:56 or a brain organoid of that person with that genetics,

00:25:00 with that genetic code in it.

00:25:02 And you can ask, what is this genetic code doing

00:25:05 to some aspects of development of the brain?

00:25:08 And for the first time, you may come to solutions

00:25:12 like what cells are involved in autism, right?

00:25:16 So many questions around this.

00:25:17 So if you take this human stem cell

00:25:20 for that particular person with that genetic code,

00:25:23 how, and you try to build an organoid,

00:25:26 how often will it look similar?

00:25:28 What’s the, yeah, so.

00:25:31 The reproducibility?

00:25:33 Yes, or how much variability is the flip side of that?

00:25:36 Yeah, so there is much more variability

00:25:40 in building organoids than there is in building brain.

00:25:44 It’s really true that the majority of us,

00:25:47 when we are born as babies,

00:25:49 our brains look a lot like each other.

00:25:52 This is the magic that the embryo does,

00:25:54 where it builds a brain in the context of a body

00:25:57 and there is very little variability there.

00:26:01 There is disease, of course,

00:26:02 but in general, a little variability.

00:26:03 When you build an organoid,

00:26:07 we don’t have the full code for how this is done.

00:26:09 And so in part, the organoid somewhat builds itself

00:26:13 because there are some structures of the brain

00:26:15 that the cells know how to make.

00:26:18 And another part comes from the investigator,

00:26:21 the scientist adding to the media factors

00:26:26 that we know in the mouse, for example,

00:26:27 would foster a certain step of development,

00:26:30 but it’s very limited.

00:26:33 And so as a result,

00:26:36 the kind of product you get in the end

00:26:38 is much more reductionist,

00:26:39 is much more simple than what you get in vivo.

00:26:42 It mimics early events of development as of today,

00:26:46 and it doesn’t build very complex type of anatomy

00:26:49 and structure does not as of today,

00:26:52 which happens instead in vivo.

00:26:54 And also the variability that you see,

00:26:59 one organ to the next tends to be higher

00:27:02 than when you compare an embryo to the next.

00:27:05 So, okay, then the next question is,

00:27:07 how hard and maybe another flip side of that expensive

00:27:11 is it to go from one stem cell to an organoid?

00:27:14 How many can you build in like,

00:27:16 because it sounds very complicated.

00:27:18 It’s work definitely, and it’s money definitely,

00:27:23 but you can really grow a very high number

00:27:28 of these organoids, can go perhaps,

00:27:31 I told you the maximum,

00:27:32 they become about five millimeters in diameter.

00:27:35 So this is about the size of a tiny, tiny raisin,

00:27:40 or perhaps the seed of an apple.

00:27:43 And so you can grow 50 to 100 of those

00:27:47 inside one big bioreactors, which are these flasks

00:27:51 where the media provides nutrients for the organoids.

00:27:55 So the problem is not to grow more or less of them.

00:28:01 It’s really to figure out how to grow them in a way

00:28:06 that they are more and more reproducible,

00:28:08 for example, organoid to organoid,

00:28:09 so they can be used to study a biological process.

00:28:13 Because if you have too much variability,

00:28:15 then you never know if what you see

00:28:17 is just an exception or really the rule.

00:28:19 So what does an organoid look like?

00:28:22 Are there different neurons already emerging?

00:28:25 Is there, well, first, can you tell me

00:28:28 what kind of neurons are there?

00:28:29 Yes.

00:28:30 Are they sort of all the same?

00:28:35 Are they not all the same?

00:28:38 How much do we understand?

00:28:39 And how much of that variance, if any,

00:28:43 can exist in organoids?

00:28:45 Yes.

00:28:47 So you could grow,

00:28:49 I told you that the brain has different parts.

00:28:52 So the cerebral cortex is on the top part of the brain,

00:28:55 but there is another region called the striatum

00:28:57 that is below the cortex and so on and so forth.

00:28:59 All of these regions have different types of cells

00:29:03 in the actual brain, okay?

00:29:05 And so scientists have been able to grow organoids

00:29:08 that may mimic some aspects of development

00:29:11 of these different regions of the brain.

00:29:13 And so we are very interested in the cerebral cortex.

00:29:16 That’s the coolest part, right?

00:29:17 Very cool.

00:29:18 I agree with you.

00:29:20 We wouldn’t be here talking

00:29:22 if we didn’t have a cerebral cortex.

00:29:23 It’s also, I like to think, the part of the brain

00:29:26 that really truly makes us human,

00:29:27 the most evolved in recent evolution.

00:29:30 And so in the attempt to make the cerebral cortex

00:29:33 and by figuring out a way to have these organoids

00:29:37 continue to grow and develop for extended periods of times,

00:29:40 much like it happens in the real embryo,

00:29:42 months and months in culture,

00:29:44 then you can see that many different types of neurons

00:29:48 of the cortex appear.

00:29:50 And at some point, also the astrocytes,

00:29:52 so the glia cells of the cerebral cortex also appear.

00:29:57 What are these astrocytes?

00:30:00 The astrocytes are not neurons, so they’re not nerve cells,

00:30:03 but they play very important roles.

00:30:06 One important role is to support the neuron.

00:30:08 But of course, they have much more active type of roles.

00:30:11 They’re very important, for example, to make the synapses,

00:30:14 which are the point of contact and communication

00:30:17 between two neurons.

00:30:21 So all that chemistry fun happens in the synapses,

00:30:25 happens because of these cells?

00:30:28 Are they the medium in which?

00:30:29 It happens because of the interactions,

00:30:31 happens because you are making the cells

00:30:34 and they have certain properties,

00:30:36 including the ability to make neurotransmitters,

00:30:40 which are the chemicals that are secreted to the synapses,

00:30:43 including the ability of making these axons grow

00:30:46 with their growth cones and so on and so forth.

00:30:49 And then you have other cells around it

00:30:51 that release chemicals or touch the neurons

00:30:55 or interact with them in different ways

00:30:57 to really foster this perfect process,

00:30:59 in this case of synaptogenesis.

00:31:02 And this does happen within organoids.

00:31:05 So the mechanical and the chemical stuff happens.

00:31:09 The connectivity between neurons,

00:31:11 this in a way is not surprising

00:31:13 because scientists have been culturing neurons forever.

00:31:18 And when you take a neuron, even a very young one,

00:31:20 and you culture it, eventually finds another cell

00:31:23 or another neuron to talk to, it will form a synapse.

00:31:26 Are we talking about mice neurons?

00:31:28 Are we talking about human neurons?

00:31:29 It doesn’t matter, both.

00:31:30 So you can culture a neuron, like a single neuron

00:31:33 and give it a little friend and it starts interacting?

00:31:37 Yes, so neurons are able to, it sounds,

00:31:41 it’s more simple than what it may sound to you.

00:31:44 Neurons have molecular properties and structural properties

00:31:48 that allow them to really communicate with other cells.

00:31:50 And so if you put not one neuron,

00:31:53 but if you put several neurons together,

00:31:55 chances are that they will form synapses with each other.

00:32:00 Okay, great.

00:32:01 So an organoid is not a brain.

00:32:03 No.

00:32:04 But there’s some, it’s able to,

00:32:09 especially what you’re talking about,

00:32:10 mimics some properties of the cerebral cortex, for example.

00:32:15 So what can you understand about the brain

00:32:17 by studying an organoid of a cerebral cortex?

00:32:21 I can literally study all this incredible diversity

00:32:25 of cell type, all these many, many different classes

00:32:28 of cells, how are they made?

00:32:30 How do they look like?

00:32:32 What do they need to be made properly?

00:32:34 And what goes wrong if now the genetics of that stem cell

00:32:39 that I used to make the organoid came from a patient

00:32:42 with a neurodevelopmental disease?

00:32:44 Can I actually watch for the very first time

00:32:47 what may have gone wrong years before in this kid

00:32:51 when its own brain was being made?

00:32:53 Think about that loop.

00:32:54 In a way, it’s a little tiny rudimentary window

00:32:59 into the past, into the time when that brain

00:33:04 in a kid that had this neurodevelopmental disease

00:33:07 was being made.

00:33:10 And I think that’s unbelievably powerful

00:33:12 because today we have no idea of what cell types,

00:33:16 we barely know what brain regions

00:33:18 are affected in these diseases.

00:33:20 Now we have an experimental system

00:33:23 that we can study in the lab.

00:33:25 And we can ask, what are the cells affected?

00:33:28 When during development things went wrong?

00:33:31 What are the molecules among the many, many

00:33:34 different molecules that control brain development?

00:33:36 Which ones are the ones that really messed up here

00:33:39 and we want perhaps to fix?

00:33:42 And what is really the final product?

00:33:44 Is it a less strong kind of circuit and brain?

00:33:48 Is it a brain that lacks a cell type?

00:33:51 What is it?

00:33:52 Because then we can think about treatment

00:33:54 and care for these patients that is informed

00:33:59 rather than just based on current diagnostics.

00:34:02 So how hard is it to detect

00:34:04 through the developmental process?

00:34:06 It’s a super exciting tool

00:34:10 to see how different conditions develop.

00:34:15 How hard is it to detect that, wait a minute,

00:34:17 this is abnormal development.

00:34:20 Yeah.

00:34:21 How much signal is there?

00:34:24 How much of it is it a mess?

00:34:26 Because things can go wrong at multiple levels, right?

00:34:29 You could have a cell that is born and built

00:34:34 but then doesn’t work properly

00:34:36 or a cell that is not even born

00:34:38 or a cell that doesn’t interact with other cells differently

00:34:40 and so on and so forth.

00:34:42 So today we have technology

00:34:44 that we did not have even five years ago

00:34:47 that allows us to look for example

00:34:49 at the molecular picture of a cell,

00:34:52 of a single cell in a sea of cells with high precision.

00:34:56 And so that molecular information

00:34:58 where you compare many, many single cells

00:35:01 for the genes that they produce

00:35:03 between a control individual

00:35:06 and an individual with a neurodevelopmental disease,

00:35:10 that may tell you what is different molecularly.

00:35:13 Or you could see that some cells are not even made,

00:35:18 for example, or that the process of maturation

00:35:20 of the cells may be wrong.

00:35:22 There are many different levels here

00:35:25 and we can study the cells at the molecular level

00:35:29 but also we can use the organoids to ask questions

00:35:33 about the properties of the neurons,

00:35:35 the functional properties,

00:35:37 how they communicate with each other,

00:35:38 how they respond to a stimulus and so on and so forth.

00:35:41 And we may get an abnormalities there, right?

00:35:46 Detect those.

00:35:47 So how early is this work in the,

00:35:51 maybe in the history of science?

00:35:54 So, I mean like, so if you were to,

00:35:59 if you and I time travel a thousand years into the future,

00:36:05 organoids seem to be, maybe I’m romanticizing the notion

00:36:09 but you’re building not a brain

00:36:12 but something that has properties of a brain.

00:36:15 So it feels like you might be getting close to,

00:36:18 in the building process, to build this to understand.

00:36:23 So how far are we in this understanding

00:36:29 process of development?

00:36:31 A thousand years from now, it’s a long time from now.

00:36:34 So if this planet is still gonna be here

00:36:36 a thousand years from now.

00:36:38 So, I mean, if, you know, like they write a book,

00:36:41 obviously there’ll be a chapter about you.

00:36:43 That’s right, that science fiction book, today.

00:36:47 Yeah, today, about, I mean, I guess where

00:36:49 we really understood very little about the brain

00:36:52 a century ago, I was a big fan in high school

00:36:55 of reading Freud and so on, still am of psychiatry.

00:36:59 I would say we still understand very little

00:37:01 about the functional aspect of just,

00:37:04 but how in the history of understanding

00:37:07 the biology of the brain, the development,

00:37:09 how far are we along?

00:37:11 It’s a very good question.

00:37:12 And so this is just, of course, my opinion.

00:37:15 I think that we did not have technology

00:37:19 even 10 years ago or certainly not 20 years ago

00:37:23 to even think about experimentally investigating

00:37:27 the development of the human brain.

00:37:30 So we’ve done a lot of work in science

00:37:32 to study the brain or many other organisms.

00:37:35 Now we have some technologies which I’ll spell out

00:37:39 that allow us to actually look at the real thing

00:37:43 and look at the brain, at the human brain.

00:37:45 So what are these technologies?

00:37:46 There has been huge progress in stem cell biology.

00:37:50 The moment someone figured out how to turn a skin cell

00:37:54 into an embryonic stem cell, basically,

00:37:57 and that how that embryonic stem cell

00:38:00 could begin a process of development again

00:38:02 to, for example, make a brain,

00:38:04 there was a huge advance,

00:38:06 and in fact, there was a Nobel Prize for that.

00:38:08 That started the field, really,

00:38:10 of using stem cells to build organs.

00:38:14 Now we can build on all the knowledge of development

00:38:17 that we build over the many, many, many years

00:38:18 to say, how do we make the stem cells

00:38:20 now make more and more complex aspects

00:38:22 of development of the human brain?

00:38:25 So this field is young, the field of brain organoids,

00:38:28 but it’s moving faster.

00:38:30 And it’s moving fast in a very serious way

00:38:32 that is rooted in labs with the right ethical framework

00:38:35 and really building on solid science

00:38:40 for what reality is and what is not.

00:38:44 But it will go faster and it will be more and more powerful.

00:38:49 We also have technology that allows us

00:38:51 to basically study the properties of single cells

00:38:54 across many, many millions of single cells,

00:38:59 which we didn’t have perhaps five years ago.

00:39:02 So now with that, even an organoid

00:39:04 that has millions of cells can be profiled in a way,

00:39:08 looked at with very, very high resolution,

00:39:11 the single cell level to really understand

00:39:13 what is going on.

00:39:14 And you could do it in multiple stages of development

00:39:17 and you can build your hypothesis and so on and so forth.

00:39:20 So it’s not gonna be a thousand years.

00:39:22 It’s gonna be a shorter amount of time.

00:39:25 And I see this as sort of an exponential growth

00:39:29 of this field enabled by these technologies

00:39:33 that we didn’t have before.

00:39:34 And so we’re gonna see something transformative

00:39:36 that we didn’t see at all in the prior thousand years.

00:39:41 So I apologize for the crazy sci fi questions,

00:39:44 but the developmental process is fascinating

00:39:48 to watch and study, but how far are we away from

00:39:53 and maybe how difficult is it to build

00:39:57 not just an organoid, but a human brain from a stem cell?

00:40:02 Yeah, first of all, that’s not the goal

00:40:05 for the majority of the serious scientists

00:40:07 that work on this because you don’t have to build

00:40:12 the whole human brain to make this model useful

00:40:16 for understanding how the brain develops

00:40:17 or understanding disease.

00:40:20 You don’t have to build the whole thing.

00:40:22 So let me just comment on this, fascinating.

00:40:25 It shows to me the difference between you and I

00:40:29 as you’re actually trying to understand

00:40:31 the beauty of the human brain and to use it

00:40:34 to really help thousands or millions of people

00:40:36 with disease and so on, right?

00:40:38 From an artificial intelligence perspective,

00:40:41 we’re trying to build systems that we can put in robots

00:40:45 and try to create systems that have echoes

00:40:49 of the intelligence about reasoning about the world,

00:40:52 navigating the world.

00:40:53 It’s different objectives, I think.

00:40:56 Yeah, that’s very much science fiction.

00:40:57 Science fiction, but we operate in science fiction a little bit.

00:41:00 So on that point of building a brain,

00:41:03 even though that is not the focus or interest, perhaps,

00:41:06 of the community, how difficult is it?

00:41:08 Is it truly science fiction at this point?

00:41:11 I think the field will progress, like I said,

00:41:13 and that the system will be more and more complex

00:41:17 in a way, right?

00:41:18 But there are properties that emerge from the human brain

00:41:23 that have to do with the mind,

00:41:25 that may have to do with consciousness,

00:41:26 that may have to do with intelligence or whatever

00:41:29 that we really don’t understand

00:41:31 even how they can emerge from an actual, real brain.

00:41:35 And therefore, we can now measure or study in an organoid.

00:41:39 So I think that this field, many, many years from now,

00:41:43 may lead to the building of better neural circuits

00:41:48 that really are built out of understanding

00:41:50 of how this process really works.

00:41:52 And it’s hard to predict how complex this really will be.

00:41:57 I really don’t think we’re so far from,

00:42:00 it makes me laugh, really.

00:42:01 It’s really that far from building the human brain.

00:42:05 But you’re gonna be building something

00:42:07 that is always a bad version of it,

00:42:11 but that may have really powerful properties

00:42:14 and might be able to respond to stimuli

00:42:18 or be used in certain context.

00:42:21 And this is why I really think

00:42:23 that there is no other way to do this science,

00:42:25 but within the right ethical framework,

00:42:28 because where you’re going with this is also,

00:42:31 we can talk about science fiction and write that book,

00:42:34 and we could today,

00:42:36 but this work happens in a specific ethical framework

00:42:41 that we don’t decide just as scientists,

00:42:43 but also as a society.

00:42:44 So the ethical framework here is a fascinating one,

00:42:48 is a complicated one.

00:42:49 Yes.

00:42:51 Do you have a sense, a grasp

00:42:53 of how we think about ethically of building organoids

00:43:00 from human stem cells to understand the brain?

00:43:04 It seems like a tool

00:43:06 for helping potentially millions of people cure diseases

00:43:11 or at least start the cure by understanding it.

00:43:14 But is there more, is there gray areas

00:43:17 that we have to think about ethically?

00:43:22 Absolutely.

00:43:23 We must think about that.

00:43:25 Every discussion about the ethics of this

00:43:29 needs to be based on actual data

00:43:32 from the models that we have today

00:43:34 and from the ones that we will have tomorrow.

00:43:36 So it’s a continuous conversation.

00:43:37 It’s not something that you decide now.

00:43:39 Today, there is no issue really.

00:43:42 Very simple models that clearly can help you in many ways

00:43:47 without much think about,

00:43:49 but tomorrow we need to have another conversation

00:43:52 and so on and so forth.

00:43:53 And so the way we do this

00:43:54 is to actually really bring together constantly

00:43:57 a group of people that are not only scientists,

00:44:00 but also bioethicists, the lawyers, philosophers,

00:44:03 psychiatrists and so on,

00:44:04 psychologists and so on and so forth

00:44:06 to decide as a society really what we should

00:44:13 and what we should not do.

00:44:15 So that’s the way to think about the ethics.

00:44:17 Now, I also think though, that as a scientist,

00:44:21 I have a moral responsibility.

00:44:23 So if you think about how transformative it could be

00:44:29 for understanding and curing a neuropsychiatric disease,

00:44:34 to be able to actually watch and study

00:44:37 and treat with drugs the very brain

00:44:40 of the patient that you are trying to study.

00:44:43 How transformative at this moment in time this could be.

00:44:47 We couldn’t do it five years ago,

00:44:48 we could do it now, right?

00:44:50 If we didn’t do it.

00:44:51 Taking a stem cell of a particular patient.

00:44:52 Patient and make an organoid for a simple

00:44:56 and different from the human brain,

00:44:58 it still is his process of brain development

00:45:02 with his or her genetics.

00:45:04 And we could understand perhaps what is going wrong.

00:45:08 Perhaps we could use as a platform,

00:45:09 as a cellular platform to screen for drugs,

00:45:12 to fix a process and so on and so forth, right?

00:45:15 So we could do it now, we couldn’t do it five years ago.

00:45:18 Should we not do it?

00:45:20 What is the downside of doing it?

00:45:24 I don’t see a downside at this very moment.

00:45:27 If we invited a lot of people,

00:45:30 I’m sure there would be somebody who would argue against it.

00:45:33 What would be the devil’s advocate argument?

00:45:37 Yeah, yeah.

00:45:39 So it’s exactly perhaps what you alluded at

00:45:42 with your question,

00:45:44 that you are enabling some process of formation of the brain

00:45:51 that could be misused at some point,

00:45:54 or that could be showing properties

00:45:59 that ethically we don’t wanna see in a tissue.

00:46:03 So today, I repeat, today, this is not an issue.

00:46:07 And so you just gain dramatically from the science without,

00:46:11 because the system is so simple and so different

00:46:15 in a way from the actual brain.

00:46:17 But because it is the brain,

00:46:19 we have an obligation to really consider all of this, right?

00:46:23 And again, it’s a balanced conversation

00:46:27 where we should put disease and betterment of humanity

00:46:30 also on that plate.

00:46:32 What do you think, at least historically,

00:46:35 there was some politicization,

00:46:37 politicization of embryonic stem cells,

00:46:44 a stem cell research.

00:46:46 Do you still see that out there?

00:46:49 Is that still a force that we have to think about,

00:46:53 especially in this larger discourse

00:46:55 that we’re having about the role of science

00:46:57 in at least American society?

00:47:00 Yeah, this is a very good question.

00:47:03 It’s very, very important.

00:47:04 I see a very central role for scientists

00:47:08 to inform decisions about what we should

00:47:12 or should not do in society.

00:47:14 And this is because the scientists

00:47:16 have the firsthand look and understanding

00:47:20 of really the work that they are doing.

00:47:23 And again, this varies depending on

00:47:26 what we’re talking about here.

00:47:27 So now we’re talking about brain organoids.

00:47:31 I think that the scientists need to be part

00:47:33 of that conversation about what is,

00:47:36 will be allowed in the future

00:47:37 or not allowed in the future to do with the system.

00:47:40 And I think that is very, very important

00:47:43 because they bring the reality of data to the conversation.

00:47:48 And so they should have a voice.

00:47:51 So data should have a voice.

00:47:53 Data needs to have a voice.

00:47:55 Because in not only data,

00:47:57 we should also be good at communicating

00:48:01 with non scientists, the data.

00:48:04 So there has been often time,

00:48:06 there is a lot of discussion and, you know,

00:48:11 excitement and fights about certain topics

00:48:16 just because of the way they are described.

00:48:19 I’ll give you an example.

00:48:20 If I called the same cellular system

00:48:23 we just talked about a brain organoid,

00:48:27 or if I called it a human mini brain,

00:48:30 your reaction is gonna be very different to this.

00:48:34 And so the way the systems are described,

00:48:37 I mean, we and journalists alike need to be a bit careful

00:48:42 that this debate is a real debate and informed by real data.

00:48:46 That’s all I’m asking.

00:48:47 And yeah, the language matters here.

00:48:49 So I work on autonomous vehicles

00:48:51 and there the use of language

00:48:53 could drastically change the interpretation

00:48:56 and the way people feel about

00:48:58 what is the right way to proceed forward.

00:49:01 You are, as I’ve seen from a presentation, you’re a parent.

00:49:06 I saw you show a couple of pictures of your son.

00:49:09 Is it just the one?

00:49:11 Two.

00:49:12 Two.

00:49:13 Son and a daughter.

00:49:13 Son and a daughter.

00:49:14 So what have you learned from the human brain

00:49:17 by raising two of them?

00:49:20 More than I could ever learn in the lab.

00:49:24 What have I learned?

00:49:25 I’ve learned that children really have

00:49:27 these amazing plastic minds, right?

00:49:30 That we have a responsibility to, you know,

00:49:34 foster their growth in good, healthy ways.

00:49:38 That keep them curious, that keeps them adventurous,

00:49:41 that doesn’t raise them in fear of things.

00:49:45 But also respecting who they are,

00:49:47 which is in part, you know,

00:49:49 coming from the genetics we talked about.

00:49:51 My children are very different from each other

00:49:53 despite the fact that they’re the product of

00:49:55 the same two parents.

00:49:58 I also learned that what you do for them comes back to you.

00:50:03 Like, you know, if you’re a good parent,

00:50:05 you’re gonna, most of the time,

00:50:08 have, you know, perhaps a decent kids at the end.

00:50:11 So what do you think, just a quick comment,

00:50:12 what do you think is the source of that difference?

00:50:16 That’s often the surprising thing for parents.

00:50:20 Is that they can’t believe that our kids,

00:50:23 oh, they’re so different,

00:50:26 yet they came from the same parents.

00:50:28 Well, they are genetically different.

00:50:29 Even they came from the same two parents

00:50:31 because the mixing of gametes,

00:50:33 you know, we know this genetics,

00:50:35 creates every time a genetically different individual,

00:50:39 which will have a specific mix of genes

00:50:43 that is a different mix every time from the two parents.

00:50:46 And so they’re not twins.

00:50:50 They are genetically different.

00:50:52 Even just that little bit of variation,

00:50:55 because you said really from a biological perspective,

00:50:58 the brains look pretty similar.

00:51:00 Well, so let me clarify that.

00:51:02 So the genetics you have, the genes that you have,

00:51:05 that play that beautiful orchestrated symphony

00:51:08 of development, different genes

00:51:12 will play it slightly differently.

00:51:13 It’s like playing the same piece of music,

00:51:16 but with a different orchestra and a different director.

00:51:20 The music will not come out.

00:51:21 It will be still a piece by the same author,

00:51:25 but it will come out differently

00:51:27 if it’s played by the high school orchestra

00:51:28 instead of the Scala in Milan.

00:51:34 And so you are born superficially with the same brain.

00:51:39 It has the same cell types,

00:51:41 similar patterns of connectivity,

00:51:43 but the properties of the cells

00:51:45 and how the cells will then react to the environment

00:51:47 as you experience your world will be also shaped

00:51:51 by who genetically you are.

00:51:53 Speaking just as a parent,

00:51:55 this is not something that comes from my work.

00:51:56 I think you can tell at birth that these kids are different,

00:52:01 that they have a different personality in a way, right?

00:52:05 So both is needed, the genetics,

00:52:08 as well as the nurturing afterwards.

00:52:11 So you are one human with a brain,

00:52:15 sort of living through the whole mess of it,

00:52:17 the human condition, full of love, maybe fear,

00:52:21 ultimately mortal.

00:52:24 How has studying the brain changed the way you see yourself?

00:52:27 When you look in the mirror, when you think about your life,

00:52:30 the fears, the love, when you see your own life,

00:52:33 your own mortality.

00:52:34 Yeah, that’s a very good question.

00:52:38 It’s almost impossible to dissociate some time for me.

00:52:43 Some of the things we do or some of the things

00:52:46 that other people do from,

00:52:48 oh, that’s because that part of the brain

00:52:52 is working in a certain way.

00:52:54 Or thinking about a teenager,

00:52:59 going through teenage years and being at time funny

00:53:02 in the way they think.

00:53:03 And impossible for me not to think it’s because

00:53:07 they’re going through this period of time

00:53:09 called critical periods of plasticity

00:53:13 where their synapses are being eliminated here and there,

00:53:16 and they’re just confused.

00:53:17 And so from that comes perhaps a different take

00:53:22 on that behavior, or maybe I can justify it scientifically

00:53:27 in some sort of way.

00:53:29 I also look at humanity in general,

00:53:32 and I am amazed by what we can do

00:53:36 and the kind of ideas that we can come up with.

00:53:39 And I cannot stop thinking about how the brain

00:53:43 is continuing to evolve.

00:53:46 I don’t know if you do this,

00:53:47 but I think about the next brain sometimes.

00:53:49 Where are we going with this?

00:53:50 Like, what are the features of this brain

00:53:53 that evolution is really playing with

00:53:57 to get us in the future, the new brain?

00:54:02 It’s not over, right?

00:54:04 It’s a work in progress.

00:54:06 So let me just a quick comment on that.

00:54:09 Do you think there’s a lot of fascination

00:54:14 and hope for artificial intelligence

00:54:15 of creating artificial brains?

00:54:17 You said the next brain.

00:54:20 When you imagine over a period of a thousand years,

00:54:23 the evolution of the human brain,

00:54:25 do you sometimes envisioning that future

00:54:28 see an artificial one, artificial intelligence,

00:54:32 as it is hoped by many, not hoped,

00:54:34 thought by many people would be actually

00:54:37 the next evolutionary step in the development of humans?

00:54:40 Yeah, I think in a way that will happen, right?

00:54:45 It’s almost like a part of the way we evolve.

00:54:48 We evolve in the world that we created,

00:54:51 that we interact with, that shape us as we grow up

00:54:55 and so on and so forth.

00:54:58 Sometime I think about something that may sound silly,

00:55:00 but think about the use of cell phones.

00:55:04 Part of me thinks that somehow in their brain,

00:55:07 there will be a region of the cortex

00:55:09 that is attuned to that tool.

00:55:13 And this comes from a lot of studies

00:55:16 in modern organisms where really the cortex,

00:55:20 especially adapts to the kind of things you have to do.

00:55:24 So if we need to move our fingers in a very specific way,

00:55:28 we have a part of our cortex that allows us

00:55:30 to do this kind of very precise movement.

00:55:34 An owl that has to see very, very far away

00:55:36 with big eyes, the visual cortex, very big.

00:55:39 The brain attunes to your environment.

00:55:43 So the brain will attune to the technologies

00:55:47 that we will have and will be shaped by it.

00:55:51 So the cortex very well may be.

00:55:52 Will be shaped by it.

00:55:54 In artificial intelligence, it may merge with it,

00:55:57 it may get, envelop it and adjust.

00:56:01 Even if it’s not a merge of the kind of,

00:56:04 oh, let’s have a synthetic element together

00:56:06 with a biological one.

00:56:08 The very space around us, the fact, for example,

00:56:11 think about we put on some goggles of virtual reality

00:56:15 and we physically are surfing the ocean, right?

00:56:18 Like I’ve done it.

00:56:19 And you have all these emotions that come to you.

00:56:22 Your brain placed you in that reality.

00:56:27 And it was able to do it like that

00:56:29 just by putting the goggles on.

00:56:31 It didn’t take thousands of years of adapting to this.

00:56:35 The brain is plastic.

00:56:37 So adapts to new technology.

00:56:39 So you could do it from the outside

00:56:41 by simply hijacking some sensory capacities that we have.

00:56:47 So clearly over recent evolution,

00:56:51 the cerebral cortex has been a part of the brain

00:56:53 that has known the most evolution.

00:56:55 So we have put a lot of chips

00:56:58 on evolving this specific part of the brain.

00:57:02 And the evolution of cortex is plasticity.

00:57:05 It’s this ability to change in response to things.

00:57:10 So yes, they will integrate.

00:57:12 That we want it or not.

00:57:14 Well, there’s no better way to end it, Paola.

00:57:18 Thank you so much for talking today.

00:57:19 You’re very welcome.

00:57:20 This is very exciting.